UniversityEssayServices

—Where does value come from? I propose a new

answer to this classic question. People experience regula-

tory fit when the manner of their engagement in an activity

sustains their goal orientation or interests regarding that

activity. When there is fit, people engage more strongly in

what they are doing and ‘‘feel right’’ about it. Fit influences

the strength of value experiences—how good or how bad

one feels about something—independently of the pleasure

and pain experiences that are associated with outcomes. It

uniquely contributes to people’s experience of the value of

things. Fit is shown to influence judgments and decision

making, attitude and behavior change, and task per-

formance.

KEYWORDS—value; evaluation; activity engagement; deci-

sion making; attitude change; performance

What makes people value something? A classic answer is that

values are socialized shared beliefs about ideal objectives and

allowable procedures for attaining them (e.g., Merton, 1957). A

second answer relates value to usefulness and emphasizes need

satisfaction rather than beliefs. For example, Adam Smith’s

(1994/1776) prime example of an object with high value was

water. From the time of the Ancient Greeks, a third answer has

emphasized value as experience, especially hedonic experi-

ences of pleasure and pain. In this paper, I present evidence for a

type of value experience separate from hedonic experiences: the

regulatory fit that people experience when the manner of their

engagement in an activity sustains their goal orientation or in-

terests regarding that activity. I propose that all value is an ex-

perience of the attraction toward or repulsion from something

whose intensity is a function of the strength of two factors—the

strength of hedonic pleasure or pain and the strength of en-

gagement (see Higgins, 2005). Regulatory fit contributes to

value through increasing strength of engagement. It makes

people ‘‘feel right’’ about whatever they are doing, and thus has a

broad influence on judgments and decision making, attitude and

behavior change, and task performance.

REGULATORY FIT

When people pursue a goal, they begin with some motivational

orientation, some concerns or interests that direct the goal

pursuit. They pursue the goal in some manner, some method or

way of executing the goal pursuit. Finally, they experience or

anticipate experiencing some desirable or undesirable outcomes

of successful or unsuccessful goal pursuit. In traditional moti-

vational models, the means or manner in which a goal is pursued

can be valued because, as a way to attain the desired outcomes, it

is either socially prescribed (e.g., having value from fulfilling

social norms of fairness), effective (i.e., having instrumental

value), or efficient (i.e., having low costs). What is valued about

the means per se is their contribution to attaining desirable

outcomes (high benefits and low costs). This value of the manner

of goal pursuit as a function of its influence on outcomes has

received substantial attention in the literature. In contrast, the

value of the manner of goal pursuit as a function of its influence

on the motivational orientation of the actor has received rela-

tively little attention.

In an earlier paper (Higgins, 2000), I proposed that people

experience regulatory fit when the manner of their engagement

in an activity sustains (rather than disrupts) their current moti-

vational orientation or interests. Fit makes people engage more

strongly in what they are doing and feel right about it. Individ-

uals, for example, can pursue the same goal with different

orientations and in different ways. Consider, for instance, stu-

dents in the same course who are working to attain an A. Some

students have a promotion-focus orientation toward an A; that is,

the goal is experienced as a hope and an ideal, as something that

satisfies the need for accomplishment. Others have a prevention-

focus orientation toward an A; the goal in this case is experienced

as a responsibility or an ‘‘ought,’’ as something that satisfies the

need for security. To pursue their goal, some students read ma-

terial beyond the assigned readings—an eager way to attain an

A—whereas others are careful to fulfill all course require-

ments—a vigilant way to attain an A. Previous studies have

found that an eager manner fits a promotion focus better than it

fits a prevention focus, whereas the reverse is true for a vigilant

manner (Higgins, 2000).

For all students, receiving an A in a course has certain out-

come benefits regardless of the orientation and manner in which

Address correspondence to E. Tory Higgins, Department of Psy- chology, Schermerhorn Hall, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027; e-mail: [email protected].

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Volume 14—Number 4 209Copyright r 2005 American Psychological Society

they pursue their goal. Independent of this outcome value,

however, there is an additional experience from regulatory fit.

Specifically, when people pursue a goal in a manner that fits their

orientation (e.g., eagerly if they have a promotion focus; vigi-

lantly if they have a prevention focus), they experience their

engagement in that goal pursuit more strongly than they do when

pursuing the goal in a way that is at odds with their orientation

(e.g., pursuing a goal eagerly if their orientation is more pre-

ventative). When the manner of their goal pursuit fits their ori-

entation they also experience a stronger evaluative reaction to

the activity (Higgins, 2000). Regulatory fit makes them feel right

about both their positive reactions to things and their negative

reactions to things, such as feeling right about one’s positive

response to some advertisement or feeling right about one’s

negative response to some other advertisement (see Cesario,

Grant, & Higgins, 2004).

In one set of studies, for example, my colleagues and I (Hig-

gins, Idson, Freitas, Spiegel, & Molden, 2003) first measured

participants’ chronic or habitual orientations to pursuing goals

(i.e., promotion or prevention). Participants were then told that,

over and above their usual payment for participating, they could

choose between a coffee mug and a pen as a gift. (Pretesting

indicated that people liked both objects but the mug was clearly

preferred.) The means of making the decision was manipulated

through framing: Half of the participants were told to think about

what they would gain by choosing the mug or the pen (an eager

strategy), and the other half were told to think about what they

would lose by not choosing the mug or the pen (a vigilant strat-

egy). As expected, almost all participants chose the coffee mug.

These participants were then asked either to assess the price of

the chosen mug or to offer a price to buy it. Participants whose

habitual orientations fit the way they were encouraged to make

the decision (promotion–eager; prevention–vigilant) gave a 40 to

60% higher price for the mug than participants in the non-fit

conditions (promotion–vigilant; prevention–eager). Consistent

with the proposal that fit strengthens evaluative reactions to

something independent of hedonic experiences per se, the fit

effect was found to be independent of the participants’ reports of

how positive or negative they felt when making their decision.

The effect was also independent of participants’ perception of

the efficiency (ease) and effectiveness (instrumentality) of the

means they used to make their choice.

Fit effects can be found for other orientations as well. For

example, we (Avnet and Higgins, 2003) experimentally induced

either a locomotion orientation, which is concerned with

movement from state to state, or an assessment orientation,

which is concerned with making comparisons. The participants

chose one book light from a set of book lights using either a

progressive-elimination strategy (i.e., eliminate the worst al-

ternative at each phase until only one alternative remains, which

fits a locomotion orientation) or a full-evaluation strategy (i.e.,

make comparisons among all of the alternatives for all of the

attributes and then choose the one with the best attributes

overall, which fits an assessment orientation). As shown in

Figure 1, the participants offered more of their own money to buy

the same chosen book light in the fit conditions (assessment–full

evaluation; locomotion–progressive elimination) than in the

non-fit conditions. In addition, this fit effect was independent of

the participants’ positive or negative feelings at the time that

they offered to buy the book light.

JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING

How does fit create value? When fit makes people feel right

about what they are doing—such as feeling right while choosing

between the mug and the pen—this experience may transfer to

feeling right about something else they do later, such as feeling

right when positively evaluating the monetary worth of the

chosen mug. The feeling-right experience from an earlier source

would be mistakenly transferred to a later evaluative response. If

so, then drawing attention to the true source of the feeling-right

experience before making the later evaluation should reduce or

eliminate the fit effect. In another mug study (see Higgins et al.,

2003), participants were reminded of the strategy they had used

to choose the mug and were told that sometimes using certain

strategies to pursue goals can make people feel right about their

goal pursuit. They were asked, ‘‘How much do you ‘feel right’

about your goal pursuit?’’; then they priced the mug. As pre-

dicted, the fit effect was eliminated (see also Cesario et al.,

2004).

There is evidence that regulatory fit influences past (retro-

spective) or future (prospective) evaluations as well. Camacho,

Higgins, and Luger (2003), for example, had participants think

back to a time in their lives when they had a conflict with an

authority figure (e.g., a parent) and it was that authority figure

$4.60

$5.89

$6.55

$4.00

$2.00

$3.00

$4.00

$5.00

$6.00

$7.00

$8.00

Assessment Locomotion

M O

N E

Y O

F F

E R

E D

Progressive Elimination Full Comparison

ORIENTATION

Fig. 1. Amount of money offered to buy a book light as a function of participants’ regulatory mode orientation (assessment or locomotion) and the type of strategy (progressive elimination or full comparison) used to decide which light to purchase. After Avnet & Higgins, 2003.

210 Volume 14—Number 4

Value From Regulatory Fit

who determined the manner of conflict resolution. The partici-

pants were asked to recall different kinds of resolution. Some

participants recalled a resolution where the authority figure

encouraged them to succeed (the pleasant–eager condition),

whereas other participants remembered a resolution where the

authority figure safeguarded them against anything that might go

wrong (the pleasant–vigilant condition). Independent of whether

the manner of resolution was itself pleasant or painful, and in-

dependent of their own pleasant or painful mood while making

their judgments, participants judged the resolution to be more

morally ‘‘right’’ in the fit conditions (promotion participants–

eager conflict resolution; prevention participants–vigilant con-

flict resolution).

In making decisions, people imagine the pleasure or pain of

the outcomes that particular choices will produce. Imagining

making a desirable choice has higher fit for people in a promo-

tion focus than it does for those in a prevention focus (because

success maintains eagerness but reduces vigilance); the oppo-

site is true for imagining making an undesirable choice (because

failure maintains vigilance but reduces eagerness). In a study by

Idson, Liberman, and Higgins (2004), participants were asked to

imagine how ‘‘good’’ or ‘‘bad’’ they would feel either paying in

cash or with a credit card to buy a book for school where the book

costs less if you pay in cash. As shown in Figure 2, participants

felt better when they imagined paying less for the book. In ad-

dition, when imagining paying less they felt even better if they

were in a promotion focus than if they were in a prevention focus;

and when imagining paying more, they felt even worse if they

were in a prevention focus than if they were in a promotion focus.

ATTITUDE AND BEHAVIOR CHANGE

A standard method to change people’s attitudes and behavior is to

provide information about the positive outcomes or benefits of

such a change. Regulatory fit permits another method for change.

If people respond positively to a message, creating fit should make

them feel right about their response and increase message effec-

tiveness. Spiegel, Grant-Pillow, and Higgins (2004), for example,

had participants read either a promotion-framed or prevention-

framed health message that contained the same information urg-

ing them to eat more fruits and vegetables. The consequences were

framed as either the benefits of complying or the costs of not

complying. Participants in the fit conditions (promotion–benefits;

prevention–costs) ate about 20% more fruits and vegetables over

the following week than those in the non-fit conditions. In a study

by Cesario et al. (2003), the fit experience was induced separately

from the message itself so that all participants could be given

exactly the same message. The participants were told that they

were performing two unrelated tasks. In the first task, fit or non-fit

was induced by having participants list either eager or vigilant

strategies to attain either promotion or prevention goals. In the

second task, all of the participants were given exactly the same

persuasive policy message. Cesario et al. (2003) found that fit

induced in the first task increased persuasion in the second task

for participants who had positive thoughts about the message but

decreased persuasion for participants who had negative thoughts

(see Fig. 3). Thus, when fit makes people feel right about their

response to an object or event, this increases the strength of that

response, whether the response is positive or negative.

TASK PERFORMANCE

According to the theory of regulatory fit (Higgins, 2000), regu-

latory fit increases strength of engagement. Consistent with this

6.7

−3.1

4.8

−5.1

−7

−5

−3

−1

1

3

5

7

PAY LESS PAY MORE

G O

O D

/B A

D F

E E

L IN

G S

PROMOTION

PREVENTION

Fig. 2. Good/bad feelings about a textbook purchase as a function of participants’ regulatory focus (promotion or prevention) and the valence of prospective outcome (paying less or more for the book). After Idson, Liberman, and Higgins (2004).

5.6

4.1

4.7

5.4

3

3.5

4

4.5

5

5.5

6

6.5

7

POSITIVE NEGATIVE

THOUGHT VALENCE

O V

E R

A L

L O

P IN

IO N

FIT NON-FIT

Fig. 3. Overall opinion of an advocated policy (higher numbers indicate higher agreement) as a function of positive vs. negative thoughts about the policy message and fit versus non-fit in a prior, unrelated task. After Cesario et al., 2003.

Volume 14—Number 4 211

E. Tory Higgins

prediction, Förster, Higgins, and Idson (1998) found that en-

gagement, as measured by either persistence on an anagram task

or arm-pressure intensity while doing the task, was stronger

in the fit conditions (promotion–eager; prevention–vigilant)

whether the regulatory focus of the participants varied chroni-

cally or had been induced experimentally. If regulatory fit

strengthens engagement, then it has the potential to improve

performance. Indeed, Förster et al. (1998) found that fit enhanced

task performance (see also Bianco, Higgins, and Klem, 2003).

Spiegel et al. (2004) also found that fit helps to bridge the gap

between intentions and action. Participants were asked to write a

report on how they were going to spend their upcoming Saturday

and to turn it in by a certain deadline to receive an extra payment

for participating in the study. The participants were also asked to

imagine how they were going to write the report, and were asked

to make either eager plans or vigilant plans for writing the report.

Performance was measured by whether or not the report was

handed in on time. Promotion participants performed better

when they prepared eager plans of when, where, and how to do

the report rather than vigilant plans. The reverse was true for

prevention participants. Indeed, participants in the fit condi-

tions (promotion–eager steps; prevention–vigilant steps) were

almost 50% more likely to turn in their reports than participants

in the non-fit conditions.

CONCLUSION

This article describes a source of value that has received little

prior attention: regulatory fit. When people make decisions or

pursue goals, they experience the value of actual or anticipated

outcomes that they find attractive or repulsive, but this is not the

only way that value is created. Making decisions and pursuing

goals are themselves activities and, as with any activity, people

can be more or less engaged in the activity. When engagement is

strong it intensifies people’s feelings about an activity, whether

those feelings are positive or negative. Regulatory fit creates

value by increasing strength of engagement. People experience

regulatory fit when the manner in which they engage in an ac-

tivity sustains their current orientation. The increased strength

of engagement produced by fit is experienced as feeling right

about what one is doing, including feeling right about one’s

evaluative reactions to objects and events in the world. Fit makes

people feel right about both their positive responses to things

and their negative responses to things. Moreover, it does this not

only for the activity that itself produced the fit, but also for later,

separate activities. My colleagues and I (Higgins et al., 2003),

for example, first had participants think about strategies for

pursuing their personal goals that either fit or did not fit their goal

orientation. A few minutes later we had participants rate nice-

looking, photographed dogs. Participants in the prior fit condi-

tions rated the dogs as more ‘‘good-natured’’ overall than those in

the prior non-fit conditions did. These results suggest that fit

induced in the first phase of the study made participants feel

right about their later positive reactions to the dogs, thereby

increasing the intensity of their positive reactions.

Fit influences the strength of value experiences, how good or

how bad one feels about something, independent of the pleasure

and pain experiences that are associated with outcomes. Other

process properties, such as the efficiency, effectiveness, or the

hedonic quality of a process, can have value beyond goal out-

comes, but fit has been shown to have effects on value that are

independent of these other process properties. It is precisely

because fit affects value through increasing strength of en-

gagement rather than producing pleasure or pain itself that its

effects are likely to go unnoticed. Failure to recognize its effects

could be problematic when fit from one source is unknowingly

transferred to the monetary value of something else or, even

worse, to the ethical value of something else, such as a public

policy program that is judged to be morally right simply because

how it is executed ‘‘feels’’ right.

Fit has significant implications for improving quality of life. In

interpersonal and intergroup conflicts, for example, it is well

recognized that the negotiation process needs to be fair, just, and

equitable. But fit also needs to be considered. By ensuring that

all parties experience fit in how they carry out a negotiation,

satisfaction with and commitment to the agreement will in-

crease, independent of outcomes. That is the good news. The bad

news is that other parties not directly involved in the negotiation,

who miss the fit experience, are unlikely to value the agreement

as much and might not support it. It is also important for parents

and teachers to allow children their own fit experiences in goal

pursuit and decision making and not simply provide them with

answers and positive outcomes. More generally, for people to

value their lives fully they need to go beyond pleasant outcomes

and feel right about what they are doing.

Recommended Reading Freitas, A.L., & Higgins, E.T. (2002). Enjoying goal-directed action:

The role of regulatory fit. Psychological Science, 13, 1–6.

Higgins, E.T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.

Higgins, E.T., Idson, L.C., Freitas, A.L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D.C.

(2003). Transfer of value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1140–1153.

Acknowledgments—The research reported in this article was

supported by Grant 39429 from the National Institute of Mental

Health to E. Tory Higgins.

REFERENCES

Avnet, T., & Higgins, E.T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regu-

latory fit: Value transfer from ‘‘how’’ to ‘‘what.’’ Journal of Exper- imental Social Psychology, 39, 525–530.

212 Volume 14—Number 4

Value From Regulatory Fit

Bianco, A.T., Higgins, E.T., & Klem, A. (2003). How ‘‘fun/importance’’

fit impacts performance: Relating implicit theories to instructions.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1091–1103.

Camacho, C.J., Higgins, E.T., & Luger, L. (2003). Moral value transfer

from regulatory fit: ‘‘What feels right is right’’ and ‘‘what feels wrong is wrong.’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 498–510.

Cesario, J., Grant, H., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). Regulatory fit and per-

suasion: Transfer from ‘‘feeling right.’’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 388–404.

Förster, J., Higgins, E.T., & Idson, C.L. (1998). Approach and avoidance

strength as a function of regulatory focus: Revisiting the ‘‘goal

looms larger’’ effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1115–1131.

Higgins, E.T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.

Higgins, E.T. (2005). Value from hedonic experience and engagement. Unpublished manuscript, Columbia University.

Higgins, E.T., Idson, L.C., Freitas, A.L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D.C.

(2003). Transfer of value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1140–1153.

Idson, L.C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). Imagining how you’d

feel: The role of motivational experiences from regulatory fit.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 926–937.

Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper.

Merton, R.K. (1957). Social theory and social structure. Glencoe, IL: The Free Press.

Smith, A. (1994). The wealth of nations. New York: Random House. (Original work published in 1776).

Spiegel, S., Grant-Pillow, H., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). How regulatory fit

enhances motivational strength during goal pursuit. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 39–54.

Volume 14—Number 4 213

E. Tory Higgins

Found something interesting ?

• On-time delivery guarantee
• PhD-level professional writers
• Free Plagiarism Report

• 100% money-back guarantee
• Absolute Privacy & Confidentiality
• High Quality custom-written papers

Related Model Questions

Feel free to peruse our college and university model questions. If any our our assignment tasks interests you, click to place your order. Every paper is written by our professional essay writers from scratch to avoid plagiarism. We guarantee highest quality of work besides delivering your paper on time.

Sales Offer

Coupon Code: SAVE25 to claim 25% special special discount
SAVE